Incubation delays territory defence by male blue-headed vireos, Vireo solitarius Ioana Chiver*, , , Eugene S. Morton† and Bridget J.M. Stutchbury*
†Smithsonian Institution Conservation and Research Center, Front Royal, VA, U.S.A.
*Department of Biology, York University, Toronto, Canada
Received 18 November 2005; revised 11 January 2006; accepted 1 June 2006. MS. number: A10304R. Available online 2 November 2006. Animals often face trade-offs during reproduction between activities such as parental care and territory defence. In species that are socially monogamous, males and females are faced with the additional problem of coordinating their respective contributions to such activities. Here, we examined male incubation behaviour in a passerine that shows genetic monogamy and in which males incubate extensively. Male incubation is rare in North American passerines and little is known about how males defend their territories during the incubation stage and whether females compensate for lower male incubation during territorial intrusions. Blue-headed vireo males contribute 50% of incubation time during the day. We performed playbacks to simulate intrusions to males while they were on the nest and off the nest during the incubation period. On average, males took 18 min to arrive at the playback location while on the nest incubating, but took less than 2 min when not incubating. In addition, only 44% of the males sang while on the nest, whereas all males sang in response to the playback when off the nest. This result suggests that males probably delay territory defence until females return to the nest to avoid exposing the nest to predators. Females returned to the nest to relieve their mates from incubation duties sooner during experimental intrusions than during control periods, which also allowed males to pursue intruders sooner. Genetic monogamy may underlie this apparent cooperation and the sex-role convergence exemplified in this species. We first evaluated two general hypotheses based on studies of different bird species reported in the literature. The hypotheses are not mutually exclusive, but they represent the two most commonly proposed functions of T in male birds, at least during the breeding period. In both cases, we modified the predictions of the hypotheses to address nonbreeding social behaviour in our own study species, P. pubescens. One hypothesis is the ‘resource defence’ hypothesis, which states that males with elevated T during winter can occupy and defend a territory to a greater degree than other males. Furthermore, expansion of the home range could lead to an increased opportunity for males to interact with nonmate females, who may be higher in quality than the current mate (cf. Raouf et al. 1997). We predicted that under the resource defence hypothesis, T-males would expand their home range, occupy space more exclusively of other males, associate with more females over time and interact more frequently with other males than would controls. T-implants in the spring have these effects in other species (Silverin 1980; Hegner & Wingfield 1987; Wingfield & Farner 1993; Ketterson et al. 1996). As a consequence of these T-influenced effects, males also neglect their mate and offspring (Ketterson et al. 1996; Hunt et al. 1999; Schwagmeyer et al. 2005). Therefore, a secondary prediction we made under the resource defence hypothesis is that T-males and T-females will associate with each other less often than control pairs do. Mate association frequencies among experimental pairs may also be low if T-males become more aggressive towards their mate. Little is known about the effects of T on male–female aggression in birds, but male woodpeckers are known to displace females from high-quality foraging sites when resources are scarce (Kilham 1970, 1974). Thus, T-males could show higher mate-displacement rates than controls, and the foraging rates of T-females could be reduced because of their efforts to avoid displacements by their mates. A second hypothesis is the ‘pair bond’ hypothesis. Testosterone might improve a male’s potential for future reproductive success by facilitating investment in a current or future pair bond rather than, or in addition to, an expanded home range and resource base (e.g. Enstrom et al. 1997; Alonso-Alvarez 2001; Peters 2002). The hormonal state of one partner affects that of the other (Wingfield & Farner 1993; Wingfield & Monk 1994), and hormonal synchrony early in the breeding season improves reproductive success (Hirschenhauser et al. 1999). Thus, in species in which mates have the opportunity to interact in winter, higher winter T levels could hasten the start (or increase the frequency) of male–female interactions. Under the pair bond hypothesis, we predicted that T-pairs would show a higher mate association frequency and greater home range overlap between mates than would controls. We further predicted that T-females would have greater foraging efficiency than C-females as a consequence of more frequent mate association. Studies on avian pair behaviour during winter have documented male protection of females against predators and conspecific food competitors (P. pubescens: Sullivan 1984, 1985; Kellam 2003; other examples: Lens & Dhondt 1993; Hogstad 1995; Fusani et al. 1997). As males become more vigilant when near the mate, female foraging efficiency improves. This helps prevent mate loss by lowering the risks of female starvation and depredation. Both the resource defence and pair bond hypotheses are based on the findings of studies conducted on a variety of bird species, usually during the breeding period. However, the results that we report here for P. pubescens in winter do not tend to support either a priori hypothesis, even though our treatment groups showed several significant behavioural differences. We therefore conclude our paper posing a third hypothesis for the function of T in males during nonbreeding periods: the mate-guarding hypothesis. We use our results to suggest that T may help males in the nonbreeding period prevent mate loss stemming from female-initiated mate switches or desertion before mating. This hypothesis contrasts with the pair bond hypothesis, which predicts that males and females would benefit similarly from a nonbreeding social relationship.
[此贴子已经被作者于2007-12-28 9:52:54编辑过] |