- UID
- 703710
- 在线时间
- 小时
- 注册时间
- 2011-12-21
- 最后登录
- 1970-1-1
- 主题
- 帖子
- 性别
- 保密
|
沙发
楼主 |
发表于 2014-8-19 23:00:51
|
只看该作者
Part II: Speed
Man-made quakes shake the ground less than natural ones
BY Alexandra Witze | Aug 18, 2014
[Time 2]
Expanded oil and gas operations in the central and eastern United States have triggered earthquakes as large as magnitude 5.7, as drillers inject wastewater back into the ground. But seismologists now report a bit of good news: such 'induced' quakes appear to shake the ground less than a naturally occurring earthquake of the same magnitude would.
That is good news because less shaking means less damage. “Maybe induced earthquakes aren’t quite as fearsome as they may seem,” says Susan Hough, a seismologist at the US Geological Survey (USGS) in Pasadena, California, whose work is published in the Bulletin of the Seismological Society of America1. “The hazard from induced quakes is going to be down a notch relative to tectonic quakes.”
But the observation holds only for areas more than 10 kilometres from the earthquake’s epicentre. Anyone close to the drilling would still feel as much shaking as a natural quake would bring. “This might lead to a recommendation that deep injection wells should be kept 10 kilometres away from population centres,” says Hough.
The injection of wastewater during conventional oil and gas extraction tends to trigger earthquakes of about magnitude 4 to 52. (The controversial technique of hydraulic fracturing, or ‘fracking’, also generates earthquakes but usually only of around magnitude 3 in size.) Some of these quakes have caused significant damage, such as a series of three quakes in November 2011 near Prague, Oklahoma, that occurred within just a few kilometers of fluid injection wells and destroyed at least 14 homes in neighbouring towns3.
[254 words]
[Time 3]
Ground truth
“There’s been an awful lot of work on induced quakes, but people haven’t really been looking at the shaking they generate,” says Hough. So she decided to explore a USGS database known as the Did You Feel It? system, in which anyone who experiences the ground moving can report it. The sheer number of people who participate makes the database an accurate and useful tool, says Hough — especially in the central and eastern United States, which are not as densely studded with seismometers as, say, California.
Hough analysed data for 11 induced earthquakes in the central and eastern United States, from February 2011 in Arkansas to December 2013 in Oklahoma. “The observations are very straightforward — in every single case the intensities are low,” she says.
The relatively low levels of shaking suggest that induced earthquakes have a low 'stress drop', a measure of how an earthquake behaves. Two earthquakes of the same magnitude can have different stress drops. Imagine two trucks rumbling across the same distance: one moves in short, fast jerks; the other moves slowly and smoothly. The second truck would have the lower stress drop.
Induced earthquakes may have lower stress drop than natural ones because the fluids injected into the ground lubricate geological faults and allow them to slip more smoothly, Hough says. The discovery may provide another tool to determine whether an earthquake is natural or induced.
Danielle Sumy, a seismologist at the University of Southern California in Los Angeles, says that Hough’s findings mesh with her own analysis of the Prague earthquakes. Sumy is analysing data from seismometers set out after the earthquakes there to capture the aftershocks. Her preliminary findings support the idea that induced quakes cause less shaking than natural quakes, Sumy says.
“[Hough] shows that what we think should happen during a natural tectonic quake isn’t really occurring for these induced events,” Sumy says. “That's the power of using community-based information.”
[321 words]
Source: Nature
http://www.nature.com/news/man-made-quakes-shake-the-ground-less-than-natural-ones-1.15742
NIH to probe racial disparity in grant awards
US agency will assess whether grant reviewers are biased against minority applicants.
BY Sara Reardon | August 19, 2014
[Time 4]
Richard Nakamura, director of the Center for Scientific Review at the US National Institutes of Health (NIH), does not consider himself to be racially biased. Yet a test of his speed at associating certain words with faces of different races revealed a slight unconscious prejudice against minorities. If the director of the institute that oversees the NIH’s grant process harbours these inclinations, he wonders, are grant reviewers affected as well?
To answer that question, the NIH will launch ambitious analyses beginning in September to determine whether bias hampers minority scientists who seek agency funding. A 2011 study in Science found that white researchers receive NIH grants at nearly twice the rate that African American researchers do (see ‘Grant gap’). Even when factors such as publication record and training are considered, an African American scientist is still only two-thirds as likely as a white scientist to be funded (D. K. Ginther et al. Science 333, 1015–1019; 2011). The disparity seems to arise early during the review process, when grants are first rated.
The findings spurred the NIH to launch a ten-year, US$500-million effort in 2012 to train and mentor minority scientists. But officials acknowledge that the racial gap among grantees is not just because there are fewer qualified applications from minority researchers. Now the agency will look inward to determine where its grant process may be failing — and what to do about it.
One basic issue that the NIH will address is whether grant reviewers are thinking about an applicant’s race at all, even unconsciously. A team will strip names, racial identification and other identifying information from some proposals before reviewers see them, and look at what happens to grant scores. (Such identity stripping is surprisingly difficult: even citations might reveal who the applicant is, and reviewers need some information about an applicant to make a fair appraisal.) The results could be telling. “If the disparity drops with anonymization, that’s clear evidence of bias,” says Nakamura.
[326 words]
[Time 5]
Such a finding would be in line with other results in this area. A study published this year found that faculty members in US universities are less likely to respond to interview requests from prospective students whose names are associated with minority groups than they are to identical requests from students with ‘white’ names (K. L. Milkman et al. Soc. Sci. Res. Network http://doi.org/t9h; 2014).
The NIH will also study reviewers’ work in finer detail, by analysing successful applications for R01 grants, the NIH’s largest funding programme for individual investigators. The goal is to see whether researchers can spot trends in the language used by reviewers to describe proposals put forward by applicants of different races. There is precedent for detectable differences: in a paper to be published in Academic Medicine, a team led by Molly Carnes, a physician at the University of Wisconsin-Madison, used automated text analysis to show that reviewers’ critiques of R01 grant applications by women tended to include more words denoting praise, as though the writer is surprised at the quality of the work. And numerous other studies show that different standards exist for men and women in a variety of fields. “Women do, indeed, have to be twice as good to get the same competence rating as a man,” says Carnes.
The NIH will also analyse text in samples of reviewers’ unedited critiques. The Center for Scientific Review typically edits the wording and grammar of these reviews before grant proposals are returned to applicants, but even the subtlest details of such raw comments might hold clues about bias. Nakamura says that reviewers will not be told whether their comments will be analysed, because that in itself would bias the sample. “We want them to be sloppy,” he says.
[293 words]
[Time 6]
The NIH’s Study Sections, in which review groups discuss the top 50% of grant applications, might also harbour bias: the 2011 Science paper found that submissions authored by African Americans are less likely to be discussed in the meetings. But when they are, a negative comment arising from even one person’s unconscious bias could have a major impact in such a group setting, says John Dovidio, a psychologist at Yale University in New Haven, Connecticut, and a member of the NIH’s Diversity Working Group. “That one person can poison the environment,” he says.
Even if the NIH investigation does not turn up evidence of bias, it may still reveal some of the causes of the racial disparity in the NIH’s grant-making process. Perhaps grants from minority researchers are more likely to be written in a way that does not appeal to reviewers, says Monica Basco, executive secretary of the Diversity Working Group’s peer-review subcommittee. That would suggest fixes such as grant-writing help. Evidence of bias would be harder to address, and any interventions would need to be tailored to address the point at which it occurs, says Basco.
Nakamura expects that the NIH’s effort to identify and root out prejudice, which he says could cost up to $5 million over three years, might prove controversial. “People resent the implication they might be biased,” he says — an idea borne out by some responses to his 29 May blogpost on the initiative. One commenter wrote, “It is absolutely insulting to be accused of review bigotry. Please tell me why I should continue to give up my time to perform peer review?”
But Nakamura believes that the NIH — and reviewers — need to keep open minds. After all, he says, “we are human beings with emotions and feelings we’re not in control of”.
[299 words]
Source: Nature
http://www.nature.com/news/nih-to-probe-racial-disparity-in-grant-awards-1.15740 |
本帖子中包含更多资源
您需要 登录 才可以下载或查看,没有帐号?立即注册
x
|